HOME
DV NEWS
SERVICE ARCHIVE SUBMISSIONS/CONTACT ABOUT DV
Demolishing
Compassion
Greed, Profit, and Animals as ‘Converting Machines’
by
David Edwards and Media Lens
May
15, 2003
You
would not be mistaken if you have noticed that there is a certain harshness
about mainstream corporate journalists, their reporting and their responses to
readers. Anyone who has worked for large corporations, as we have, knows that
compassion, kindness and concern for others find little space in the bottom
line accounting of corporate managers. Noam Chomsky indicates the reality:
“The chairman of the board may sincerely
believe that his every waking moment is dedicated to serving human needs. Were
he to act on these delusions instead of pursuing profit and market share, he
would no longer be chairman of the board.” (Chomsky, Necessary Illusions: Thought
Control in Democratic Societies, Pluto Press, 1991, p.19)
What
kind of impact do these bottom line framing conditions have on the kind of
personnel recruited to work in this environment? And what kind of behaviour
does it promote in them and in society? Psychologist Erich Fromm discussed the
problem with reference to a fictional merchant who he imagined experiencing the
curious impulse to give away goods free of charge to someone in need:
"We live in a society that is
directed toward success and profit, and not one that is founded on love. Thus,
the person who acts out of a sense of love excludes himself from social
thinking; he becomes an outsider. The merchant in our example [of experiencing
but repressing the desire to be compassionate and generous] can hardly tell his
wife about this because she would call him an 'idiot'. Even less can he tell his
colleagues about it; he would lose his credibility and be considered half
feeble-minded." (Fromm, quoted The Essential Fromm, ed, Rainer
Funk, pp.124-5)
Many
readers may themselves have winced at Fromm’s reference to a society “founded
on love”. As we will argue below, concern for others directly conflicts with
the inherent greed and violence of modern capitalism, and is therefore deeply
opposed by the political and economic centres of power that influence and shape
our culture. Well-meaning readers occasionally advise us not to even discuss
these issues on the grounds that “this may alienate some of your audience. By
broadening the scope of your work is there not a risk that your current success
regarding the media may be diluted?”
There
is indeed, but we believe it is vital to recognise the fundamental importance
of the choice between greedy self-obsession and concern for others in
determining the extent to which people collude with, or defy, attempts to
subordinate people and planet to profit. The Vietnamese peace activist, Thich
Nhat Than, writes of a social activist, Yasodhara, who lived around 500 BC,
recognising that she (and we) are not driven merely by political ideas:
“People were entrapped not only by
illness and unjust social conditions, but by the sorrows and passions they
themselves created in their own hearts and minds. And if in time, Yasodhara
fell victim to fear, anger, bitterness, or disappointment, where would she find
the energy needed to continue her work?” (Thich Nhat Than, Old Path White
Clouds, Routledge, 1991, p.66)
The
reluctance to discuss exactly where activists are supposed to find the energy
to continue their work in response to these problems is one of the great
failings of modern dissent. Many writers focus almost entirely on politics and
economics, on facts and figures, as if there were nothing more to us - feeling,
suffering human beings though we are.
Is
it not obvious, for example, that it is the sincerity and intensity of their
concern for others that separates the likes of Chomsky, Herman, Zinn, Beder,
Pilger, Curtis, and Roy from their mainstream counterparts? It seems clear to
us that it is their refusal to suppress their doubts and criticisms in exchange
for money and prestige their shame at even the thought that they might do so
at other people’s expense that separates these writers from the mainstream.
Their
compassion, we believe, is rooted in three inter-related factors: first, a
basic rational honesty contained in the understanding that our personal
interests are not more important than the interests of others - we are
only one individual, after all, whereas other people and animals are
numberless. We cannot credibly justify subordinating the interests of others to
the needs of this single self. Second, it is rooted in the ability to feel the
misery of others as real and important, to not be so totally swallowed up by
our own goals and desires that we are numb to their pain.
Hidden
away alongside this rational honesty and emotional sensitivity is another
factor promoting compassion the sense that reining-in self-concern and
increasing our concern for others is also conducive to our own well-being.
Focusing solely on our own needs places those needs which are, after all,
problems firmly at the centre of our psychological universe. Like objects
under a magnifying glass, to the extent and intensity that we focus on these
problems, the larger and more important they seem to us. Fromm commented on the
common mistake in response:
“They [patients in therapy] think the
best way to cure oneself is the complete concentration on one’s own problems.
But that is +not+ the best way it’s the worst way.” (Fromm, The Art of
Listening, Constable & Company, 1994, p.166)
Indeed,
it makes sense that placing other people’s problems at the centre of our
concerns some of the time means that our own problems thereby come to seem
smaller, less significant. Compassion for others thus has the effect of
actually removing or reducing problems in our mind. In his book, Destructive
Emotions, psychologist Daniel Goleman reports research by Richard Davidson,
director of the Laboratory for Affective Neuroscience Behaviour at the
University of Wisconsin, which suggests that intensive and sincere concern for
others does indeed have positive effects:
“The very act of concern for others'
well-being, it seems, creates a greater sense of well-being within
oneself." (Goleman, Disturbing Emotions - And How We Can Overcome Them,
Bloomsbury, 2003, p.12)
This
has very real implications for human happiness. Cultures around the world have
long warned of the hidden dangers of a self-obsessed life and the hidden
benefits of compassion. Aryadeva made a witty observation about the nature of
desire, for example:
“If
desire were pleasurable
There
would be no need for women [or men].
Pleasure
is not regarded as
Something
to get rid of.”
If
desire is in itself uncomfortable, then so is acting on desire whether we are
successful or not - as the Chinese philosopher, Hsing Yun, notes:
“When
one seeks an object of desire,
one
suffers.
When
one gets an object of desire,
One
fears losing it.
When
one loses an object of desire,
One
is greatly troubled.
At
each and every point,
There
is no joy.”
By
contrast, as Khenpo Karthar Rinpoche notes, working for the happiness of others
has an extraordinarily beneficial effect:
“Come to an understanding that no matter
how it may seem, the root of all suffering is in actuality the desire to
accomplish our own benefit and our own aims, and the root of all happiness is
the relinquishment of that concern and the desire to accomplish the benefit of
others.”
It
is a remarkable claim, but one worth investigating by anyone who has tried
devoting their energy to the alternative without success. Note that this claim
is the absolute antithesis of everything our culture needs us to believe -
devoted, as it is, to the promotion of unrestrained greed and endlessly rising
consumption. This means that many of us have plenty of trained resistance to
it, and it means we are unlikely ever to encounter it in the mainstream.
The
suppression of compassion in Western culture is sometimes so extreme that it
verges on the surreal. For much of Western scientific history animals have, for
example, been dismissed as clockwork mechanisms to be abused at will.
Descartes, among many others, declared animals “thoughtless brutes”, arguing
that it was mere sentimentalism to credit them with actual feelings. An unknown
contemporary of Descartes described one result of this conviction:
“The [Cartesian] scientists administered
beatings to dogs with perfect indifference and made fun of those who pitied the
creatures as if they felt pain. They said the animals were clocks; that the
cries they emitted when struck were only the noise of a little spring that had
been touched, but that the whole body was without feeling. They nailed the poor
animals up on boards by their four paws to vivisect them to see the circulation
of the blood...” (Quoted, Masson and McCarthy, When Elephants Weep,
Vintage, 1996, p.33)
The
curious conviction that animals are without feelings has gone largely
unchallenged to this day, as Jeffrey Masson and Susan McCarthy note in their
book When Elephants Weep: The Emotional Life of Animals:
“So persistent are the forces that
militate against admitting the possibility of emotions in the lives of animals
that the topic seems disreputable, not a respectable field of study, almost
taboo.” (Ibid, p.20)
To
be sure, this is not for lack of evidence:
“The scholarly literature on animals contains
many observations, accounts, anecdotes and stories that suggest interpretation
in terms of the emotions the animals may be experiencing or expressing, or call
for further research into this possibility. Little to none is forthcoming.”
(Ibid, p.20)
The
issue has simply been ignored. Of the research that might have been carried
out, Masson and McCarthy report simply that “there is almost none”. Modern
society has been happy to passively accept centuries-old dogma as “common sense
fact”. But why? Surely it could not be through a lack of scientific
objectivity, through the influence on scientists of vast economic and political
forces requiring a particular view? The answer is almost as obvious as the
question:
“Dominant human groups have long defined
themselves as superior by distinguishing themselves from groups they are
subordinating. Thus whites define blacks in part by differing melanin content
of the skin; men are distinguished from women by primary and secondary sex
characteristics. These empirical distinctions are then used to make it appear
that it is the distinction themselves, not their social consequences, that are
responsible for the social dominance of one group over the other.” (Ibid, p.21)
If
you wonder why so many of us cringe at even the mention of the word
‘compassion’ consider the display of conditional humanitarian concern
represented by the 1954 Protection of Birds Act, which states:
“If any person keeps or confines any bird
whatsoever in any cage or their receptacle which is not sufficient in height,
length or breadth to permit the bird to stretch its wings freely, he shall be
guilty of an offence against the Act and be liable to a special penalty.”
(Quoted, Danny Penman, The Price of Meat, Gollancz, 1996, p.82)
These
fine words are followed by a small proviso: “Provided that this subsection
shall not apply to poultry.”
Why
the exception? The answer is provided by the briefest of glances at the
factory-farming system.
More
than 700 million chickens are currently reared and killed in Britain each year.
A standard modern broiler unit consists of four sheds, with the floor of each
carpeted by some 30-40,000 birds. For efficiency, today’s broilers have been
designed to grow at twice the rate of 30 years ago. Their legs and hearts,
however, have not. The result is that the chicken rapidly outgrows its skeletal
strength such that its legs literally break under the weight; crippling joint
pains and other skeletal problems are inevitably legion.
Research
published in 1992 in the Veterinary Record reported that 90 per cent of birds
had detectable abnormalities in walking; in about 26 per cent of cases birds
were likely to have suffered chronic pain. The hearts and lungs of broilers are
similarly stressed by their rapid body growth: the Agriculture and Food
Research Council estimates that about seven million birds simply drop down dead
for this reason every year. About two and a half million birds die while being
‘harvested’ for slaughter, with half dying of heart failure and a third from
physical injuries: many birds have their femurs dislocated at the hip as the
result of being carried by ‘catchers’ ‘harvesting’ them by one leg. This
generally causes internal bleeding and, in a third of cases, actually drives
the bone up into the abdomen.
The
attitude required by
the poultry industry in general was summed up by the Farmer and Stockbreeder in
1982:
“The modern layer is, after all, only a
very efficient converting machine, changing the raw material feedingstuffs -
into the finished product - the egg.” (Ibid, p.82)
As
for pigs:
“Forget the pig is an animal. Treat him
just like a machine in a factory. Schedule treatments like you would
lubrication. Breeding season like the first step in an assembly line. And
marketing like the delivery of finished goods.” (Hog Farm Management, quoted,
Peter Goering, Helena Norberg-Hodge and John Page, From the Ground Up,
Zed Books, 1993, p.25)
We
might recoil in horror at the experiments conducted in Descartes’ time and
imagine that we have entered a new, enlightened age. Alas, consider last
month’s report by ITN’s science editor, Lawrence McGinty. ITN, like the BBC on
the same day, discussed research by the Roslin Institute and the University of
Edinburgh suggesting that fish had the capacity to experience pain. McGinty
said:
“Anglers are seething about this
research... Anglers are very concerned that this will be used as ammunition
against them. Especially when only last month there was another set of research
published in America showing that the brains of fish are actually so primitive,
so undeveloped, that they can’t feel pain in any sense, directly contradictory
to the research published today. So I think this is a thorny question to which
science doesn’t yet have the answer.” (ITN Lunchtime News, April 30, 2003)
Over
on the BBC, Jon Kay reported:
“Today, we have the first scientific
study” showing fish feel pain. “Britain’s anglers are angry they insist their
sport is not cruel and fish do not suffer when they’re caught.”
Kay
went on:
“The research has been seized upon by the
animal rights lobby they’ve long claimed that fishing is just as cruel as
hunting and shooting.” But anglers “say there has been plenty of research done
to disprove these latest findings”. (BBC 1 O’Clock News, April 30, 2003)
The
research referred to by both McGinty and Kay, suggesting that fish do not feel
pain, was published in February of this year by James Rose, an angler, of
Wyoming University, and is the first to draw this conclusion. Max Gastone,
co-ordinator for the Campaign for the Abolition of Angling said:
"This is pure spurious science. Mr
Rose has been hawking these claims since 2001, and they simply don't float. He
is obviously biased in favour of his sport. We wonder why he has not had his
work independently examined by people in his own specialist field, as opposed
to publishing in a journal dedicated to general coverage of fisheries
issues." (Press Release, ‘Anti-angling group dismisses new 'study' on pain
in fish as spurious’, February 10, 2003)
The
BBC’s claim that the latest research was “the first scientific study” showing
that fish feel pain is flatly false. Studies from the University of Utrecht in
the 1970s showed that fish appear to feel the pain of electric shocks in a way
comparable to that experienced by humans:
“That the perceptions evoked in fish by
local electrical stimulation do parallel to some degree the similarly produced
sensations of pain in man is indicated by the results of other experiments. We
have found, for instance, that conspicuous transitions in fish behaviour on the
one hand, and in sensations of human subjects on the other hand, tend to occur
at comparable levels of electrical stimulation in either species.” (‘Do Pain
And Fear Make A Hooked Carp In Play Suffer?’, by Prof.dr.F.J. Verheijen &
Dr.R.J.A. Buwalda, published April 1988)
An
RSPCA report concluded in 1994:
“These
studies show that fish... avoid noxious stimuli, show a reluctance to resubmit
themselves to noxious stimuli, and learn to associate neutral stimuli with
painful stimuli, indicating that fish are indeed capable of feeling pain.”
(Kestin, S.C., ‘Pain and Stress in Fish’, Bristol: Royal Society of the
Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, 1994).
In
1980, the RSPCA-sponsored Medway Report was published, which concluded that
"all vertebrates (including fish), through the mediation of similar
neuropharmacological processes, experience similar sensations to a greater or
lesser degree in response to noxious stimuli." This led the RSPCA to adopt
a policy stating:
"The
RSPCA believes that current practices in angling do involve infliction of pain
and suffering on fish." (RSPCA Policies on Animal Welfare: Bloodsports,
1991).
It
seems remarkable that anyone can still seriously claim that fish are incapable
of feeling pain. In an article titled, ‘The Moral Standing Of Insects and The
Ethics of Extinction’, published in the Florida Etymologist in 1987, the
authors reported that the answer to the question: “Can Insects Feel Pain?” was
a resounding “Yes”. This conclusion was supported by C.H. Eisemann et al, in
‘Do Insects Feel Pain? A Biological Review’, published in Experientia in 1984.
Some
may find the issue of animal suffering of marginal importance, and yet this
suffering is the result of the same logic at work at the heart of all
capitalist operations: Third World nations and peoples are “converting
machines” changing human and natural resources into profit. The corporate mass
media system of newspapers, TV, radio, books, magazines and films is a
“converting machine” changing the raw experiences and possibilities of human
existence into a web of business-friendly delusions and deceptions.
Parliamentary politics is a “converting machine” changing the raw human desire
for genuine happiness and freedom into a “moderate” desire to select from
profit-friendly options. Schools are “converting machines” processing human
beings into obedient producers and consumers - and so.
Mainstream
culture has a vested interest in suppressing compassion for people and animals
beyond our immediate circle of family and friends. The point is that
self-seeking greed and compassion are opposed. Vested interests, such as
advertisers, want us locked into desire mode, thinking primarily of ourselves,
working hard to earn, buy and consume. The last thing our profit-maximising
system wants is teenagers concerned about civilian victims of bombing in Iraq,
or tortured animals in our farming system. Compassion, therefore, has to be
ridiculed as ‘naïve’ and ‘sentimental’.
If,
as has been claimed by many of the world’s most sophisticated cultures and
philosophies, compassion is the fundamental root of human well-being, then it
is reasonable to argue that modern capitalism is organised to generate profit
and power at the expense of human happiness.
David Edwards is the editor of Media Lens, and the author of Burning All
Illusions: A Guide to Personal and Political Freedom (South End
Press, 1996). Email: editor@medialens.org. Visit the Media
Lens website: http://www.MediaLens.org
*
Related Link: Animal
Liberation Front FAQ on animal rights and responses to common arguments against
AR